Abstract

Background. Black-White differences are reported in social, psychological, behavioral, medical, and biological correlates of depression. This study was conducted to compare Black and White older adults for the association between neuroticism polygenic risk score (N-PRS) and chronicity of depressive symptoms over 20 years.


Methods. Data came from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), 1990 – 2012, a nationally representative sample of Americans above age 50. Current analysis followed 9,249 individuals (7,924 Whites and 1,325 Blacks) for up to 22 years. Depressive symptoms were measured every two years between 1992 and 2012 using the 8-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D-8). The independent variable was N-PRS. The dependent variable was average depressive symptoms between 1992 and 2012. Linear regression was used for data analysis.


Results. In the pooled sample, higher N-PRS was associated with higher average depressive symptoms over the 20-year follow up period [b=0.01, 95%CI=0.00 to 0.04], net of all covariates. We also found an interaction between race and N-PRS [b=-0.02, 95%CI=-0.03 to 0.00], suggesting a stronger effect of N-PRS on 20-year average depressive symptoms for Whites than Blacks. Based on our race-specific linear regression models, higher N-PRS was associated with higher depressive symptoms from 1992 to 2012 for Whites [b=0.01, 95%CI=0.01 to 0.02] but not Blacks [b=0.00, 95%CI=-0.02 to 0.02].


Conclusion. Black and White older adults may differ in the salience of the existing N-PRS for depressive symptoms, which better reflects the burden of depression for Whites than Blacks. This may be because the existing PRSs are derived from mostly or exclusively White samples, limiting their applicability in other race groups. Racial variation in psychosocial, clinical, and biological correlates of depression needs further research.


Background

Neuroticism (N), a relatively stable personality trait with major public health significance,[1] reflects some of the between-individual variations in the tendency to respond to threats with negative emotion. N is of high public health significance because it is a strong predictor of a wide range of undesired physical health outcomes such as heart disease, stroke, hypertension, diabetes, and obesity.[2,3] N is also associated with an increased risk of premature mortality.[3] In addition, N predicts quality of life, health service use, and mental disorders including depression.[1-3]

N is believed to reflect vulnerability to anxiety and depression.[4-8] Higher scores of N predict more frequency and intensity of negative emotional reactions in response to stress.[2] Individuals with a higher N score are also more sensitive to negative emotional information.[9,10] Therefore, individuals with higher N frequently experience emotional arousal which is a well-accepted risk factor for a wide range of negatively charged emotions (e.g., sadness, anger, anxiety, fear, worry, frustration, distress, loneliness, and depression).[11-13]

N, however, may not be universally harmful. The health consequences of N may depend on the context, culture, and health outcome, probably through differential behavioral and physiological consequences of N trait across subpopulations.[14-17] At high-risk environments, high N may help individuals avoid exposures.[18] In such contexts, N may become protective, as individuals with high N may have a higher tendency to avoid risks due to their higher sensitivity to the potential costs associated with environmental exposures.[19] In this view, contextual factors such as culture and environment should be regarded as moderating factors that alter the health effects of N.[18] In a study, N was a risk and protective factor for cardiovascular mortality in women with low and high socioeconomic status (SES), respectively.[20] N differently moderates the effect of social support on health across cultures.[19] Thus, the health effects of N may depend on sociodemographic factors.

A recent study suggested that N may better reflect the future risk of depression for Whites than Blacks.[21] In the Americans’ Changing Lives (ACL) data that included 847 Whites and 372 Blacks, higher N at baseline was associated with higher risk major depressive disorder (MDD) 25 years later (odds ratio [OR] = 2.23) in the pooled sample. The study showed an interaction between race and baseline N on subsequent risk of MDD (OR = 0.37), suggesting a weaker effect for Blacks compared to Whites. Race-specific models showed an effect for Whites (OR = 2.55) but not Blacks (OR = 0.90).[21]

According to the Cultural Moderation hypothesis, correlates of N and other domains of negative affectivity depend on culture, race, and ethnicity.[22] As proposed by the “Differential Effects hypothesis,”[23] psychosocial mechanisms that shape populations’ health and illness are group-specific and not universal. As suggested by this hypothesis, such mechanisms vary across race and ethnic groups. These hypotheses conceptualize race and ethnicity as potential effect modifiers that may alter the effects of the very same risk factors on the very same health outcomes.[24] Considerable empirical data support these hypotheses,[25] as multiple Black-White differences have been found in correlates of psychosocial constructs such as negative affect, anger, and N.[22,27]

Race, ethnicity, culture, and SES may alter the salience of N as a psychological risk factor for depression.[19,20] N may not have the same salience for the majority as the minority groups.[21,28] To better understand racial and ethnic differences in the association between N polygenetic risk (N-polygenic scores [PGS]) on depressive symptoms, the current study compared Blacks and Whites aged 50 or older for the association between N-PGS and average depressive symptoms over a 20-year follow-up period, using a national sample of older adults in the United States.

Methods

Study design

Data were from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), 1990–2012. HRS is an ongoing state-of-the-art longitudinal cohort study with a nationally representative sample of U. S. adults over the age of 50. The HRS has collected extensive data on SES, psychological factors, health behaviors, physical health, mental health, and health-care utilization, with the primary goal of understanding the healthy transition of populations into retirement.

The baseline HRS interview was conducted in 1992. Follow-up interviews are conducted every 2 years, using alternating face-to-face and telephone interviews. Probability sampling was used to select households in all 50 states, and primary respondents were selected from age-eligible household members. Spouses or partners of primary respondents were also enrolled in the study. The core sample in the HRS was comprised people who were born between 1931 and 1941 and were 51 and 61 years of age at the time of enrollment. To maintain the sample size (due to attrition) and the representative nature of the data to the U. S. adults older than 50 years, HRS has recruited additional participants overtime and now includes over 37,000 respondents. More information on the HRS study design, measures, and methodology are available elsewhere.[29-31]

Analytic sample

This study included only White or Black individuals from the HRS core sample with data on N-polygenic risk score (N-PRS) and depressive symptoms. The White analytic sample consisted of respondents who self-reported being White and met criteria for genetically determined White ancestry (HRS quality control report for additional details);[48] the same procedure was used to create the Black analytic sample. The current analysis included 9249 individuals (7924 Whites and 1325 Blacks).

Ethics

The University of Michigan Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the HRS study protocol. All participants signed written informed consent documents. All study procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national), as determined by the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, and revised in the year 2000. Participants received financial compensation for their participating in the study.

Measures

Demographics

All demographics were taken from the baseline interview (1992). Demographic factors included sex (one for female and 0 for male), age (continuous measure in years), and race (a dichotomous variable, Whites = 0 and Blacks = 1).

SES

We took SES measures including education and income from the 1992 survey. We used continuous variables for years of education as well as household income.

Depressive symptoms

An 8-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression scale (CES-D)[33] was applied to measure the frequency of depressive symptoms.[32,33] This CES-D, applied at each wave, asked respondents about the extent to which they felt depressed, lonely, sad, and happy in the past week. All of the item responses were dichotomous. An average score was calculated with a potential range from 0 to 8. A higher score indicated more depressive symptoms.

Chronic medical conditions (CMCs)

The HRS obtained self-reported information on respondents’ CMC at all waves. Respondents were asked whether a physician has ever diagnosed them with each condition. The following seven conditions were evaluated: high blood pressure, diabetes, cancer, lung disease, heart disease, stroke, and arthritis.

Self-rated health (SRH)

HRS asked respondents’ overall health using a 5-point scale. Responses were excellent, very good, fair, good, and poor. SRH was treated as a continuous variable, with a potential range from 1 to 5, with a higher score indicating worse SRH.[34,35]

Body mass index (BMI)

From 1992 to 2004, BMI was calculated based on self-reported weight and height. BMI was then directly measured starting in 2006, during face-to-face interviews. Weight and height were originally collected in pounds (1 pound = 0.453 kg) and feet (1 foot = 0.3048 m)/inches (1 inch = 0.0254 m). As people have a tendency to overestimate their height and underestimate their weight, BMI based on self-reported data is on average an underestimation of actual BMI, particularly for women. However, BMI based on self-reported weight and height strongly correlates with directly measured BMI.[36,37]

N-PRS

Using the Illumina HumanOmni2.5 BeadChip (HumanOmni2.5-4 v1, HumanOmni2.5-8 v1), genotype data were obtained from the HRS participants, based on about 2.4 million single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). Individuals were removed if they had the first degree relatives in the HRS or missing call rates more than 2%. SNPs with call rates <98% or Hardy–Weinberg P < 0.0001 were removed. Individuals were evaluated for chromosomal anomalies, and all affected SNPs were removed from the analysis if an anomaly was detected. Genotype data were based on saliva samples DNA that was collected in 2006, 2008, or 2010.

The N-PRS was created using beta coefficients from a genome-wide association studies (GWAS) meta-analysis for neuroticism conducted on approximately 171,000 participants,[38] with the HRS sample removed. The Whites’ PRS contains 1,152,920 SNPs that overlapped between the HRS genotype data and the GWAS meta-analysis; Blacks’ PRS contains 1,148,174 SNPs. The N-PRSs were standardized within racial groups (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1).[39,40,74]

Statistical analysis

We used SPSS for Windows 21.0 (IBM Inc., Armonk, NY) for data analysis. Descriptive and frequency tables were used for univariate analysis. Four linear regression models were used for multivariable analysis. In all models, N-PRS was the main independent variable, average depressive symptoms over 20 years from 1992 to 2012 were the main outcome, and demographics (age and sex), SES (education and income), and health (CMC, SRH, and BMI), and baseline depressive symptoms were covariates. First, we ran two models in the pooled sample. Model 1 tested the main effects of N, race, and covariates. Model 2 also included an interaction term between race and N. Then, we conducted separate race-specific models for Whites (Model 3) and Blacks (Model 4). We reported unstandardized regression coefficients (b), their 95% confidence interval (CI), and P values for each variable. P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

The current analysis included 9249 individuals who were either Whites (n = 7924, 85.7%) or Blacks (n = 1325, 14.3%). Table 1 summarizes the descriptive statistics of the pooled sample and for each racial group. Blacks had lower education and income and worse physical health status (i.e., CMC, SRH, and BMI). Blacks also had higher average depressive symptoms over time compared to Whites [Table 1].

*. Descriptive statistics in the pooled sample and stratified by race
Characteristics All ( n = 9249) Whites ( n = 7924) Blacks ( n = 1325)
n % n % n
Sex
Male 3857 41.70 3374 42.58 483
Female 5392 58.30 4550 57.42 842
Mean±SD Mean±SD Mean ± SD
Age (years) 56.33±3.96 56.38±3.99 56.17±3.85
Education (years) * 12.19±3.24 12.42±3.12 11.30±3.19
Income (thousands of USD) * 225.80±485.73 265.94±525.73 70.45±136.96
Chronic medical conditions (baseline) * 0.92±1.00 0.89±0.98 1.16±1.07
Selfrated health (baseline) * 2.44±1.13 2.35±1.11 2.88±1.12
Body mass index (baseline) * 27.17±4.90 26.87±4.64 28.87±5.86
NPRS 0.01±1.01 0.01±1.01 0.03±1.01
Depressive symptoms (20year average) * 0.14±0.25 0.13±0.24 0.18±0.28
NPRS: Neuroticism polygenic risk score.

Table 2 shows correlation coefficients for the study variables in the pooled sample and based on race. In the pooled sample and in Whites, N-PRS was negatively correlated with education and income and positively correlated with CMC, SRH, BMI, and average depressive symptoms overtime. In Blacks, N-PRS was negatively correlated with education and income and positively correlated with CMC, SRH, and BMI, but not with average depressive symptoms overtime [Table 2].

Table 2. Correlation matrix in the pooled sample and based on race
Characteristics 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Pooled sample
Race (Black) 0.068 ** −0.019 −0.129 ** −0.137 ** 0.099 ** 0.168 ** 0.147 ** 0.005 0.074 **
Sex (female) 1 −0.132 ** −0.040 ** −0.047 ** 0.081 ** 0.054 ** −0.019 −0.001 0.137 **
Age (years) 1 −0.068 ** 0.045 ** 0.083 ** 0.048 ** −0.023 0.02 −0.001
Education (years) 1 0.214 ** −0.123 ** −0.352 ** −0.117 ** −0.071 ** −0.253 **
Income (USD) 1 −0.096 ** −0.167 ** −0.074 ** −0.033 * −0.112 **
Chronic medical conditions (baseline) 1 0.458 ** 0.256 ** 0.047 ** 0.251 **
Selfrated health (baseline) 1 0.218 ** 0.058 ** 0.337 **
Body mass index (baseline) 1 −0.001 0.102 **
Depressive symptoms (20year average) 1 0.081 **
NPRS 1
Racial groups
Race (Black)
Sex (female) 1 −0.131 ** −0.064 ** −0.039 ** 0.061 ** 0.033 * −0.078 ** −0.004 0.130 **
Age (years) 0.137 ** 1 0.057 ** 0.044 ** 0.096 ** 0.061 ** −0.012 0.028 0.006
Education (years) 0.142 ** −0.188 ** 1 0.205 ** −0.111 ** −0.350 ** −0.122 ** −0.077 ** −0.251 **
Income (USD) −0.070* 0.022 0.256 ** 1 −0.087 ** −0.155 ** −0.063 ** −0.036 * −0.109 **
Chronic medical conditions (baseline) 0.151 ** 0.051 −0.125 ** −0.131 ** 1 0.447 ** 0.231 ** 0.039 * 0.236 **
Selfrated health (baseline) 0.085 ** −0.009 −0.259 ** −0.111 ** 0.474 ** 1 0.201 ** 0.058 ** 0.325 **
Body mass index (baseline) 0.176 ** −0.053 0.009 0.021 0.285 ** 0.171 ** 1 0.009 0.080 **
Depressive symptoms (20year average) 0.017 −0.02 −0.05 0.013 0.083 * 0.056 −0.043 1 0.096 **
NPRS 0.150 ** −0.034 −0.216 ** −0.097 ** 0.285 ** 0.342 ** 0.113 ** 0.021 1
NPRS: Neuroticism polygenic risk score. In the lower panel, Whites are the upper diagonal and Blacks are the lower diagonal. *P<0.0, ** P<0.01, *** P<0.001

Table 3 shows two linear regression models in overall sample. These models have the N-PRS as the independent variable (predictor) and mean depressive symptoms overtime as the dependent variable (outcome). The first model only included the main effects. The second model also included an interaction term between race and N-PRS. According to the first model, in the pooled sample, a higher N-PRS was associated with a higher average of depressive symptoms over the follow-up time (b = 0.01, 95% CI = 0.00–0.04], net of all covariates. According to the second model, we found a significant interaction between race and N-PRS on average depressive symptoms (b = −0.02, 95% CI = −0.03–0.00], suggesting that the association between N-PRS and average depressive symptoms is smaller for Blacks than Whites [Table 3].

Table 3. Effect of NPRS on average of depressive symptoms over 20 years of followup in the pooled sample ( n = 9249)
Characteristics Model 1 Model 2
Main effects Model 1 + Race × NPRS
b 95% CI b 95% CI
Race (Black) 0.02 * 0.00–0.04 0.02 * 0.00–0.04
Sex (female) 0.04 *** 0.03–0.06 0.04 *** 0.03–0.06
Age (years) 0.00 0.00–0.00 0.00 0.00–0.00
Education (years) −0.01 *** −0.01–−0.01 −0.01 *** −0.01–−0.01
Income (USD) 0.00 0.00–0.00 0.00 0.00–0.00
CMC 0.03 *** 0.02–0.04 0.03 *** 0.02–0.04
SRH 0.04 *** 0.04–0.05 0.04 *** 0.04–0.05
BMI 0.00 0.00–0.00 0.00 0.00–0.00
NPRS 0.01 *** 0.01–0.02 0.01 *** 0.01–0.02
NPRS × Race −0.02 * −0.03–0.00
Intercept 0.10 # −0.01–0.21 0.10 # −0.01–0.22
CMC: Chronic medical condition, BMI: Body mass index, SRH: Selfrated health, N-PRS: Neuroticism polygenic risk score. P <0.1, * P <0.05, ** P <0.01, *** P <0.001

Table 4 reports the results of linear regressions with N-PRS as the independent variable and average depressive symptoms overtime as the dependent variable based on race. Based on these linear regression models, in Whites, higher N-PRS was associated with higher average depressive symptoms over the follow-up time (b = 0.01, 95% CI = 0.01–0.02]. In Blacks, however, N-PRS was not associated with average depressive symptoms over the follow-up time (b = 0.00, 95% CI = −0.02–0.02].

***.
Characteristics Whites ( n = 7924) Blacks ( n = 1325)
b 95% CI b 95% CI
Sex (female) 0.04 *** 0.03–0.05 0.06 ** 0.02–0.10
Age (years) 0.00 0.00–0.00 0.00 −0.01–0.00
Education (years) −0.01 *** −0.01–−0.01 −0.02 *** −0.02–−0.01
Income (USD) 0.00* 0.00–0.00 0.00 0.00–0.00
CMC 0.03 *** 0.02–0.04 0.03 ** 0.01–0.06
SRH 0.04 *** 0.03–0.05 0.06 *** 0.04–0.08
BMI 0.00 0.00–0.00 0.00 0.00–0.00
NPRS 0.01 *** 0.01–0.02 0.00 −0.02–0.02
Intercept 0.09 −0.02–0.21 0.23 −0.15–0.60
Effect of NPRS on average depressive symptoms over 20 years of followup by race

Discussion

We found that N-PRS is associated with average depressive symptoms for Whites but not Blacks age 50 or older. This finding is in line with the previous research that bio-psychosocial correlates of negative affect, MDD, and depressive symptoms are stronger for Whites than Blacks.[36,41-47] We provide four potential explanations for the current finding. The first three explanations are not specific to N. The last explanation suggests that high N may not similarly indicate depressive symptom risk for sociodemographic groups.

Our first explanation is based on how PRSs are developed and validated. As currently available PRSs are developed and validated in exclusively or almost exclusively White samples, PRSs typically explain more outcome variability in Whites. This is partially due to the discordant patterns of linkage disequilibrium across diverse populations.[48] As a result, the existing PRSs better reflect the pertaining phenotypes for Whites than other minority groups.[39,40,49,50] N-PRS is no exception to this rule. Research into the development and validation PRSs in non-White populations will show whether the same percentage of the variance of phenotypes can be explained by PRSs for Whites and racial and ethnic minorities.[40] Similar results are reported for other PRSs.[51,52]

The second explanation is a measurement argument. Many psychosocial measures show better psychometric properties in Whites compared to Blacks as they have been developed in studies that have mostly enrolled Whites. The same is true for psychometric properties of the existing N and depression measures that better operate in Whites compared to any minority groups including Blacks. The result is systematically weaker correlates of psychosocial factors for Blacks in comparison with Whites.[41-43,53,54] There is also some specific evidence showing that CES-D measure may not provide identical results for Whites and Blacks.[57-60] Relevance of DSM criteria may differ for the depression of Blacks and Whites,[55,56] resulting in different nature of depression based on race and ethnic group. Racial and ethnic groups may also differ in what constructs that are designed to capture personality traits such as N reflect.[56] This problem is not specific to N and depression, as race and ethnicity alter the meaning of almost all psychosocial measures.[61,62] Race and ethnicity alter how personality traits and psychological distress covary with particular psychiatric disorders such as MDD.[63] Psychiatric disorders better impact perceived mental health and perceived need to health care in Whites than Blacks.[64,65] Concordance between CES-D score and clinical depression also depends on contextual factors such as race and ethnicity.[66,67] Stressful life events may also better reflect the risk of MDD in Whites than Blacks.[45,46] The link between depressive symptoms and MDD also varies by race.[90] As mentioned, the stronger associations between subjective and objective health outcomes in Whites than Blacks extend to a wide range of psychosocial and health measures.[53,63,64,68]

The associations between race, SES, personality traits such as N, and health are very complex. From one side, research has shown that personality traits such as N may partially mediate the effects of SES and health. N explains some between SES strata differences in mortality risk, as well as some individual risk heterogeneity within each SES strata. As a result, N may be shaped by individual predispositions as well as social and structural inequalities.[69] At the same time, SES alters the effects of N on health. One study showed a significant interaction between sex, N, and SES on cardiovascular diseases mortality, so the effects of N were stronger in women with low SES, whereas the effects of N were smaller among high SES women.[70] Race also changes the health effects of N.[21] However, the direction of the moderation of race and low SES seems counterintuitive. One explanation is that race and SES differently engage behavioral and biological mechanisms that reflect the effects of N on physical and mental health. More research is needed in these complex patterns.

The third explanation, again not specific to N, is the unequal effects of potentials to actual outcomes due to racism in the United States. In the presence of racism, resources and assets better translate to their pertaining outcomes in Whites than Blacks. In this view, the presence or absence of risk and protective factors has more consequences for the privileged than the minority group. A similar pattern is documented for several economic resources such as education, employment, neighborhood quality, and size of social network on risk of mortality.[25,71-73] Similar patterns are also shown for psychological assets such as effect, anger control, self-efficacy, and perceived control over life.[70,74-76]

The fourth explanation is specific to N. The previous empirical research has suggested that N[19,20] and other negative affectivity measures[77-80] may have group specific rather than universal health effects. In a recent study using ACL data, high N at baseline predicted subsequent risk of clinical MDD 25 years later for Whites but not Blacks.[21] Depressive symptoms predicted all-cause[77] and cause-specific[78] mortality for Whites but not Blacks. Hostility and anger have also predicted cardiovascular mortality for Whites but not Blacks.[79] Park et al. found that N altered the link between social support and health in Japanese but not American individuals,[19] and anger may even be linked to better health in some cultures.[80] Park et al. showed that for White Americans, lower social standing was associated with greater expression of anger, but for Japanese individuals, high social status was associated with more anger expression. While for White Americans, anger expression was predicted by subjective social status, for Japanese, objective social status predicted anger expression.[22] These racial differences are important as both current and historical racism, and discrimination may have altered social and behavioral implications for N, vigilance, and sensitivity to threats for Blacks.

In line with the last explanation, Kitayama et al. have argued that N becomes a protective factor in some and a risk factor in other contexts, depending on the level of risk in the environment. As N reflects sensitivity to potential costs associated with environmental exposures, high N may be associated with avoiding exposures through vigilance.[19] At least in some contexts, high N may mean less exposure, which has health implications.[18,81] Blacks and Whites also differ in the effect of stress on chronic disease and depression, possibly due to their differential behavioral coping strategies.[82-86] Moderating effects of context, race, and ethnicity hold for a wide range of psychosocial domains and health.[26,27,77,78] This is in part due to race, ethnicity, and culture shape experiences and expression of emotions.[87-89] More research is still needed to discern the exact mechanisms by which race, ethnicity, sex, and culture influence health outcomes such as depression.

There are limitations to this study. First, this study did not measure N. Second, we measured depressive symptoms using a self-report measure rather than the risk of clinical MDD diagnosis based on structural interviews. Third, this study used an 8-item CES-D measure that may have differential validity across ethnic groups. As a result, there is a need for replication of these findings using standardized measures of depression and N. Fourth, we conceptualized N, SES, and health as fixed factors; however, all of these constructs are subject to change overtime. Fifth, the sample size was not balanced between Whites and Blacks resulting in lower statistical power for Blacks. Sixth, we did not control for mental health-care use, antidepressant prescription, or access to care. Seventh, due to the higher mortality of Blacks, race is not independent of attrition in this study. This has implications for the calculation of 20-year average of depressive symptoms. Eighth, it is not clear that the results are solely due to the interaction of N-PRS with race. Future research should test the interactions between N-PRS and SES. More research is needed on validation of these findings using a wide range of data sets. Although these limitations exist, our study was one of the first attempts to explore Black-White variation in the link between N-PRS and depression.

Conclusion

Black and White older adults differ in the salience of currently accepted genetic predisposition for N on severity of depressive symptoms. The N-PRS does not operate well for Blacks, which may be because the PRS was developed and validated in Whites. This finding is in line with other previously reported Black-White differences in social, psychological, clinical, and biological correlates of depression. Other explanations may be structural racism and the cultural moderation hypothesis.

Animal Study

None.

References

  1. Lahey BB. Public health significance of neuroticism. Am Psychol. 2009; 64 : 241-56 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  2. Thomas SP. Neuroticism: A construct that deserves the attention of mental health researchers and clinicians. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2009; 30 : 727 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  3. Shipley BA, Weiss A, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in the UK health and lifestyle survey:A 21-year prospective cohort study. Psychosom Med. 2007; 69 : 923-31 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  4. Barnhofer T, Chittka T. Cognitive reactivity mediates the relationship between neuroticism and depression. Behav Res Ther. 2010; 48 : 275-81 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  5. Farmer A, Redman K, Harris T, Mahmood A, Sadler S, Pickering A. Neuroticism, extraversion, life events and depression. The Cardiff depression study. Br J Psychiatry. 2002; 181 : 118-22 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  6. Roelofs J, Huibers M, Peeters F, Arntz A, van Os J. Rumination and worrying as possible mediators in the relation between neuroticism and symptoms of depression and anxiety in clinically depressed individuals. Behav Res Ther. 2008; 46 : 1283-9 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  7. Lam D, Smith N, Checkley S, Rijsdijk F, Sham P. Effect of neuroticism, response style and information processing on depression severity in a clinically depressed sample. Psychol Med. 2003; 33 : 469-79 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  8. Sen S, Nesse RM, Stoltenberg SF, Li S, Gleiberman L, Chakravarti A. ABDNF coding variant is associated with the NEO personality inventory domain neuroticism, a risk factor for depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003; 28 : 397-401 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  9. Derryberry D, Reed MA. Temperament and attention:Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994; 66 : 1128-39 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  10. Wilson EJ, MacLeod C, Mathews A, Rutherford EM. The causal role of interpretive bias in anxiety reactivity. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006; 115 : 103-11 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  11. Costa PT, McCrae RR. The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment: Personality Measurement and TestingSage: London; 2008.
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  12. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being:Happy and unhappy people. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980; 38 : 668-78 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  13. Rusting CL, Larsen RJ. Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative affect:A test of two theoretical models. Pers Individ Dif. 1997; 22 : 607-12 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  14. Nesse RM, Ellsworth PC. Evolution, emotions, and emotional disorders. Am Psychol. 2009; 64 : 129-39 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  15. Marks IM, Nesse RM. Fear and fitness:An evolutionary analysis of anxiety disorders. Ethol Sociobiol. 1994; 15 : 247-61 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  16. Friedman HS. Long-term relations of personality and health:Dynamisms, mechanisms, tropisms. J Pers. 2000; 68 : 1089-107 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  17. Turiano NA, Mroczek DK, Moynihan J, Chapman BP. Big 5 personality traits and interleukin-6:Evidence for “healthy neuroticism” in a US population sample. Brain Behav Immun. 2013; 28 : 83-9 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  18. Karney BR, Bradbury TN. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability:A review of theory, method, and research. Psychol Bull. 1995; 118 : 3-4 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  19. Park J, Kitayama S, Karasawa M, Curhan K, Markus HR, Kawakami N. Clarifying the links between social support and health:Culture, stress, and neuroticism matter. J Health Psychol. 2013; 18 : 226-35 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  20. Hagger-Johnson G, Roberts B, Boniface D, Sabia S, Batty GD, Elbaz A. Neuroticism and cardiovascular disease mortality:Socioeconomic status modifies the risk in women (UK health and lifestyle survey). Psychosom Med. 2012; 74 : 596-603 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  21. Assari S. Neuroticism predicts subsequent risk of major depression for whites but not blacks. Behav Sci (Basel). 2017; 7 : 64 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  22. Park J, Kitayama S, Markus HR, Coe CL, Miyamoto Y, Karasawa M. Social status and anger expression:The cultural moderation hypothesis. Emotion. 2013; 13 : 1122-31 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  23. Assari S. Race and ethnic differences in additive and multiplicative effects of depression and anxiety on cardiovascular risk. Int J Prev Med. 2016; 7 : 22 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  24. Assari S, Lankarani MM. Education and alcohol consumption among older Americans;black-white differences. Front Public Health. 2016; 4 : 67 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  25. Assari S. Unequal gain of equal resources across racial groups. Int J Health Policy Manag. 2017; 7 : 1-9 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  26. Assari S, Moghani Lankarani M. Depressive symptoms and self-esteem in white and black older adults in the United States. Brain Sci. 2018; 8 : 105 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  27. Assari S, Lankarani MM. Race and urbanity alter the protective effect of education but not incomeon mortality. Front Public Health. 2016; 4 : 100 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  28. Gale CR, Booth T, Mõttus R, Kuh D, Deary IJ. Neuroticism and extraversion in youth predict mental wellbeing and life satisfaction 40 years later. J Res Pers. 2013; 47 : 687-97 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  29. Hauser RM, Willis RJ. Aging, Health, and Public Policy:Demographic and Economic PerspectivesThe Population Council, Inc.: New York; 2005.
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  30. Heeringa SG, Connor JH. . Technical Description of the Health and Retirement Survey Sample Design. 1995 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  31. Sonnega A, Faul JD, Ofstedal MB, Langa KM, Phillips JW, Weir DR. Cohort profile:The health and retirement study (HRS). Int J Epidemiol. 2014; 43 : 576-85 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  32. Steffick DE, Documentation of Affective Functioning Measures in the Health and Retirement Study. HRS AHEAD Documentation Report. Ann ArborUniversity of Michigan: MI; 2000.
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  33. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale:A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977; 1 : 385-401 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  34. Assari S, Lankarani MM, Burgard S. Black-white difference in long-term predictive power of self-rated health on all-cause mortality in United States. Ann Epidemiol. 2016; 26 : 106-14 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  35. Assari S. Gender differences in the predictive role of self-rated health on short-term risk of mortality among older adults. SAGE Open Med. 2016; 4 : 2050312116666975 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  36. Taylor AW, Dal Grande E, Gill TK, Chittleborough CR, Wilson DH, Adams RJ. How valid are self-reported height and weight?A comparison between CATI self-report and clinic measurements using a large cohort study. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2006; 30 : 238-46 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  37. Simon GE, Von Korff M, Saunders K, Miglioretti DL, Crane PK, van Belle G. Association between obesity and psychiatric disorders in the US adult population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006; 63 : 824-30 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  38. Okbay A, Baselmans BM, De Neve JE, Turley P, Nivard MG, Fontana MA. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat Genet. 2016; 48 : 624-33 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  39. Ware EB, Schmitz LL, Faul JD, Gard AM, Smith JA, Mitchell CM. Method of Construction Affects Polygenic Score Prediction of Common Human Traits. BiorXiv the Preprint Server for Biology. 2017 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  40. Ware E, Schmitz L, Faul J, HRS Polygenic Scores 2006-2010 Genetic Data University of Michigan. Ann ArborSurvey Research Center: Michigan; 2017.
    Google Scholar 
  41. Assari S, Moghani Lankarani M. Secular and religious social support better protect blacks than whites against depressive symptoms. Behav Sci (Basel). 2018; 8 : 46 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  42. Assari S, Sonnega A, Leggett A, Pepin RL. Residual effects of restless sleep over depressive symptoms on chronic medical conditions:Race by gender differences. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities 2016. 2016 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  43. Watkins DC, Assari S, Johnson-Lawrence V. Race and ethnic group differences in comorbid major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and chronic medical conditions. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2015; 2 : 385-94 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  44. Assari S, Caldwell CH. Gender and ethnic differences in the association between obesity and depression among black adolescents. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2015; 2 : 481-93 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  45. Assari S, Lankarani MM. Stressful life events and risk of depression 25 years later:Race and gender differences. Front Public Health. 2016; 4 : 49 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  46. Assari S, Lankarani MM. Association between stressful life events and depression;intersection of race and gender. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2016; 3 : 349-56 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  47. Assari S. Social determinants of depression:The intersections of race, gender, and socioeconomic status. Brain Sci. 2017; 7 : 156 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  48. Health and Retirement Study. . Quality Control Report for Genotypic Data. 2013 .
    Google Scholar 
  49. Martin AR, Gignoux CR, Walters RK, Wojcik GL, Neale BM, Gravel S. Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. Am J Hum Genet. 2017; 100 : 635-49 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  50. Assari S, Bazargan M, Smith JA. Polygenic Risk Score and Body Mass Index in Older Adults;Race by Gender Differences in a 20 Year Cohort Study. Geriatrics. 2019 .
    Google Scholar 
  51. Assari S, Bazargan M. . Polygenic Risk Score and Depressive Symptoms of American Older Adults Over Six Years, Racial Differences. 2019 .
    Google Scholar 
  52. Khera AV, Chaffin M, Wade KH, Zahid S, Brancale J, Xia R. 52. Polygenic prediction of weight and obesity trajectories from birth to adulthood. 2019; 177 : 587-96 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  53. Jang Y, Park NS, Kang SY, Chiriboga DA. Racial/ethnic differences in the association between symptoms of depression and self-rated mental health among older adults. Community Ment Health J. 2014; 50 : 325-30 .
    Google Scholar 
  54. Canady RB, Stommel M, Holzman C. Measurement properties of the centers for epidemiological studies depression scale (CES-D) in a sample of African American and non-hispanic white pregnant women. J Nurs Meas. 2009; 17 : 91-104 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  55. Green JG, Gruber MJ, Kessler RC, Lin JY, McLaughlin KA, Sampson NA. Diagnostic validity across racial and ethnic groups in the assessment of adolescent DSM-IV disorders. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012; 21 : 311-20 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  56. Scollon CN, Diener E. Love, work, and changes in extraversion and neuroticism over time. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006; 91 : 1152-65 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  57. Assari S. High sense of mastery reduces psychological distress for African American women but not African American men. Arch Gen Intern Med. 2019; 3 : 5-9 .
    Google Scholar 
  58. Assari S, Moazen-Zadeh E. Confirmatory factor analysis of the 12-item center for epidemiologic studies depression scale among blacks and whites. Front Psychiatry. 2016; 7 : 178 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  59. Kim G, Chiriboga DA, Jang Y. Cultural equivalence in depressive symptoms in older white, black, and Mexican-American adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009; 57 : 790-6 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  60. Boutin-Foster C. An item-level analysis of the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale (CES-D) by race and ethnicity in patients with coronary artery disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008; 23 : 1034-9 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  61. Jang Y, Kwag KH, Chiriboga DA. Not saying i am happy does not mean i am not:Cultural influences on responses to positive affect items in the CES-D. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010; 65 : 684-90 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  62. Assari S. Ethnic groups differ in how poor self-rated mental health reflects psychiatric disorders. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2017 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  63. Assari S, Dejman M, Neighbors HW. Ethnic differences in separate and additive effects of anxiety and depression on self-rated mental health among blacks. J Racial Ethnic Health Disparities. 2015; : 1-8 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  64. Kim G, DeCoster J, Chiriboga DA, Jang Y, Allen RS, Parmelee P. Associations between self-rated mental health and psychiatric disorders among older adults:Do racial/ethnic differences exist?. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011; 19 : 416-22 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  65. Breslau J, Cefalu M, Wong EC, Burnam MA, Hunter GP, Florez KR. Racial/ethnic differences in perception of need for mental health treatment in a US national sample. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017; 52 : 929-37 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  66. Moazen-Zadeh E, Assari S. Depressive symptoms predict major depressive disorder after 15 years among whites but not blacks. Front Public Health. 2016; 4 : 13 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  67. Arango-Lasprilla JC, Kreutzer JS. Racial and ethnic disparities in functional, psychosocial, and neurobehavioral outcomes after brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2010; 25 : 128-36 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  68. Kim G, Bryant A, Huang C, Chiriboga D, Ma GX. Mental health among Asian American adults:association with psychiatric. Asian Am J Psychol. 2012; 3 : 44-52 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  69. Chapman BP, Fiscella K, Kawachi I, Duberstein PR. Personality, socioeconomic status, and all-cause mortality in the United States. Am J Epidemiol. 2010; 171 : 83-92 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  70. Assari S. General self-efficacy and mortality in the USA;racial differences. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2017; 4 : 746-57 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  71. Assari S. Health disparities due to diminished return among black Americans:Public policy solutions. Soc Issues Policy Rev. 2018 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  72. Assari S. Whites but not blacks gain life expectancy from social contacts. Behav Sci (Basel). 2017; 7 : 68 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  73. Assari S. Life expectancy gain due to employment status depends on race, gender, education, and their intersections. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2018; 5 : 375-86 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  74. Ware EB, Schmitz LL, Faul JD, Gard A, Mitchell C, Smith JA, Zhao W. . Heterogeneity in Polygenic Scores for Common Human Traits. .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  75. Assari S, Lankarani MM. Reciprocal associations between depressive symptoms and mastery among older adults;black-white differences. Front Aging Neurosci. 2016; 8 : 279 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  76. Assari S. Race, sense of control over life, and short-term risk of mortality among older adults in the United States. Arch Med Sci. 2017; 13 : 1233-40 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  77. Assari S, Moazen-Zadeh E, Lankarani MM, Micol-Foster V. Race, depressive symptoms, and all-cause mortality in the United States. Front Public Health. 2016; 4 : 40 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  78. Assari S, Burgard S. Black-white differences in the effect of baseline depressive symptoms on deaths due to renal diseases:25 year follow up of a nationally representative community sample. J Renal Inj Prev. 2015; 4 : 127-34 .
    Google Scholar 
  79. Assari S. Hostility, anger, and cardiovascular mortality among blacks and whites. Res Cardiovasc Med. 2016 .
    View Article    Google Scholar 
  80. Kitayama S, Park J, Boylan JM, Miyamoto Y, Levine CS, Markus HR. Expression of anger and ill health in two cultures:An examination of inflammation and cardiovascular risk. Psychol Sci. 2015; 26 : 211-20 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  81. Kitayama S, Park J, Miyamoto Y, Date H, Boylan JM, Markus HR. Behavioral adjustment moderates the link between neuroticism and biological health risk:A U.S. -Japan comparison study. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2018; 44 : 809-22 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  82. Hicken MT, Lee H, Mezuk B, Kershaw KN, Rafferty J, Jackson JS. Racial and ethnic differences in the association between obesity and depression in women. J Womens Health (Larchmt). 2013; 22 : 445-52 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  83. Jackson JS, Knight KM, Rafferty JA. Race and unhealthy behaviors:Chronic stress, the HPA axis, and physical and mental health disparities over the life course. Am J Public Health. 2010; 100 : 933-9 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  84. Jackson JS, Knight KM. Social Structures, Aging, and Self-regulation in the ElderlySpringer: New York; 2006.
    Google Scholar 
  85. Mezuk B, Abdou CM, Hudson D, Kershaw KN, Rafferty JA, Lee H. “White box” epidemiology and the social neuroscience of health behaviors:The environmental affordances model. Soc Ment Health. 2013; 3 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  86. Mezuk B, Rafferty JA, Kershaw KN, Hudson D, Abdou CM, Lee H. Reconsidering the role of social disadvantage in physical and mental health:Stressful life events, health behaviors, race, and depression. Am J Epidemiol. 2010; 172 : 1238-49 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 
  87. Mesquita B. Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001; 80 : 68-74 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  88. Imada T, Ellsworth PC. Proud Americans and lucky Japanese:Cultural differences in appraisal and corresponding emotion. Emotion. 2011; 11 : 329-45 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  89. Mesquita B, Frijda NH. Cultural variations in emotions:A review. Psychol Bull. 1992; 112 : 179-204 .
    View Article    PubMed    Google Scholar 
  90. Assari S, Moazen-Zadeh E. Ethnic variation in the cross-sectional association between domains of depressive symptoms and clinical depression. Front Psychiatry. 2016; 7 : 53 .
    View Article    PubMed    PMC    Google Scholar 

Comments & Peer Review